Neuromyths Part 3: Too Old to Learn a Language
Ever heard "this kid is a natural" when it comes to learning a language? Is there an age limit? Can one be "too old" to learn a language? Let's look at the research.
Whatever you might have heard about this, whether you can remember or not, I’m positive it’s related to one of the most famous linguistics debates, which has been around since the 1960s.
You see, the idea that there is a certain period of maximal language acquisition and that it is essential to be exposed to language before this window closes, while it is certainly true, generally speaking, is still a matter of controversy. But before we delve into the particularities of this debate, we must understand what language consists of, how we acquire or learn it, and, mainly, what this period might look like.
In the 1960s, one of the fathers of modern linguistics, Noam Chomsky (1965) proposed that humans must have some sort of internal mechanism, which he called the Language Acquisition Device (LAD), responsible for implicitly “picking up” language. That goes hand in hand with the notion of Universal Grammar (UG). According to Chomsky, languages have generalizable grammatical features and rules that are innate to humans.
Skinner (1957), on the other hand, had already proposed a theory to explain how humans acquire language through operant conditioning, specifically positive reinforcement. Basically, when a baby tries to say something and realizes that parents respond positively to that behavior and give them what they want, they do more and more of it.
Another theory is that babies “take statistics” of phonemes in their native language and start recognizing certain patterns that they use to acquire it (Kuhl et al. 2014
Although oversimplified, as it’s not the purpose of this post to fully explore second language acquisition theories, one thing is undeniable: humans have achieved an unparalleled level of complex language capable of expressing the most abstract thoughts like no other species - at least according to us, humans. Teach a dog or a monkey as much as you want, but they will never learn the language to the degree we have mastered it (some would argue they don’t even have language). Therefore, we could assume that, as in most things, nature and nurture play an important role in language learning. And nature is what interests us now.
What might be some of the biological processes behind language acquisition? A very widely accepted notion, not without its controversy, is that there is a maturational period for languages to develop. This is called the Critical Period Hypothesis (CPH) (Lenneberg, 1967). Lenneberg popularized Penfield’s (1959) work and used its influences of animal experiments to suggest that, like many other biological systems and functions, language must be acquired before a certain age or it will be severely impaired. Hubel and Wiesel (1959) had already demonstrated that cats have around a 3-month window of opportunity to develop vision when adequately exposed to environmental stimuli (in this case, light). If they cannot open their eyes during this period, they will be virtually blind for the rest of their lives.
Based on this notion of a critical period, said to end at age 12 (Lenneberg, 1967), educators, policymakers, and parents around the globe can make very important decisions about language courses and children. However, how certain is science about this critical period? How can it be tested? What about people who have successfully learned a language after this period? Well, based on this ongoing linguistics debate, let’s try to answer some of these questions below:
1. Are language acquisition and language learning the same?
You might have noticed that at times I use the word acquisition and then learning. According to Krashen, the difference is quite simple: acquisition = more implicit, subconscious effort; learning = more explicit, conscious effort. You might say then that babies and young kids mostly acquire whereas adults mostly learn a language. Learning has to do with receiving formal education. However, I will keep using these interchangeably because the focus of this blog is on Ultimate Attainment (UA), that is, how fluent and proficient one can get at the end of their language learning (or acquisition) period.
Spoiler: According to a study done in Israel (Ferman & Karni, 2010), kids use more implicit mechanisms while adults use more explicit mechanism for language learning (or should I say acquisition?). Nevertheless, Ferman and Karni found no evidence that kids are better than young adults, they actually found the opposite.
2. How long does this critical period last?
Different authors have come to different conclusions. It could be anywhere between 5 and 18 years old. Nevertheless, one thing authors seem to agree on is that there might be different critical periods for different aspects of a language. Oral competence, particularly pronunciation, for instance, might have a critical period that ends before 1-year-old (Kuhl et al. 2014). It’s worth mentioning that a very large study was conducted by Hartshorne, Tenenbaum, and Pinker (2018) on almost 700 thousand people from all over the world and they believe our ability to learn a language starts declining after 17 yo. Now, whether this decline is exclusively maturational or due to a sudden loss of interest in language learning, substantial social life change, like going to university or work, remains unclear. Also, this large study didn't test oral competence.
3. Is it impossible (or nearly impossible) to learn a language after the critical period?
Again, there’s plenty of evidence out there supporting the fact that we can learn languages at any age. Some authors, like Norrman and Bylund (1995), even prefer to use the Montessorian notion of sensitive periods instead of critical because at the end of a critical period there should be a sharp decline in the acquisition of a function, which doesn’t seem to happen for language.
4. How can researchers test it?
It would be unethical to deprive kids of language exposure for many years and then do it to study if they’re still able to acquire this language. That’s why researchers look into second language acquisition as a proxy. However, studies with feral children (Curtiss, 1977) suggest that there is a critical period for first language acquisition. Genie was a 12yo girl found locked in a room who had never learned the written or spoken language. Her father psychologically abused her and when a psychologist/linguist tried to teach her English, which would be her first language, she was unable to master it, using an excessively fragmented language filled with grammatical errors. The question is: was that because she had passed the critical period or a consequence of psychological trauma - or both?
5. What are some of the possible gaps in Second Language Acquisition research?
As we know, science has limitations and studies need to limit their scope in order to be able to establish causality - and correlation. That means that many studies don’t analyze the classic four abilities in language that we know based on the Common European Framework of Reference for Languages (CEFR). Most of the studies focused on multiple-choice grammar-reading tests and disregarded oral fluency for instance. Another issue is that these studies normally create their own language tests and don’t use any official guidelines necessarily. Of course, we must take into account that there is probably a linguist on the research team or that they referred to Johnson and Newport (1989)’s grammar test, however, one could argue that the lack of standardization is exactly what makes the results so divergent.
6. What about technology? Can it help end this debate?
Some neuroimaging studies (Andrews et al. 2013, Abutalebi, 2008) have suggested that the age of acquisition is not as important as the duration of exposure or proficiency levels. Early brain scans showed very distinct activations in the brain when the subjects used L1 versus when they used L2. However, these studies tested, for the most part, subjects that were not fluent. When advanced subjects were tested in a Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging machine (fMRI), they were able to demonstrate that both L1 and L2 use very similar neuronal networks (same areas are activated) suggesting that the ultimate attainment (fluency level) of a second language can be quite similar to L1 and, thus, depend more on level of proficiency. Nonetheless, we cannot make bold claims and say that the debate is over.
7. Is it truly harder for adults to learn a second language when compared to kids and teens?
As language teachers, we might feel like jumping to that conclusion. But we must ask ourselves: how long does it take a kid to get to the C2 level? Is it faster than an adult learner? To me, it’s quite clear that adults can learn quite quickly at the beginning (faster than kids), especially if you think of how much vocabulary and grammar they can take in throughout a semester. But kids and teens might have a couple of advantages: 1) more exposure to L2 outside the classroom; 2) less interference of L1 on L2; 3) less fear of exposure in class; 4) less say in whether they will study an L2 or not (they don’t usually have a choice). Learners who started before the end of the critical period (as kids or teens) also seem to have more native-like pronunciation. The question is: which native and is that really an advantage in today’s world of English as a Lingua Franca?
Conclusion
What research knows for a fact is that there is compelling evidence for a decline in the ability to learn languages after a certain period of our lives. This decline may have biological reasons, no doubt, but it might also be related to social aspects or, the way I see it, both. Language is also a social phenomenon and it does not depend solely on the exposure to environmental stimuli. It requires social interaction, implicit and explicit efforts, reflection, and persistence. We must also look at the other side of this story and see how many successful learners are out there who started learning a language in their 30s, 40s, 50s or older.
If there's any disadvantage to starting a second language after what might be a critical period, it seems to be mostly related to not acquiring native-like pronunciation. The thing is, I don't think having a native-like pronunciation is such a big deal. Some native speakers of English have accents that might be easier or harder to understand and life goes on. The focus should be on intelligibility, that is, we should embrace accent diversity and value people’s capability to communicate with people.
I suppose my final message here is: don’t make this critical period hypothesis become a self-fulfilling prophecy. Not starting a second language course because you believe you’re way past the time to actually learn it is probably the biggest reason why will never learn it. Don’t take it from me, though, take it from Dr. Mary Hobson, who started studying Russian at 56 because of her passion for the literature, got a Ph.D. in Russian Literature, became an official translator of Aleksander Pushkin’s work and won a medal by the Russian government in recognition of her excellent work.
Learning a language is possible at any age and it might feel more difficult as you get older because of a number of reasons. The thing is: it probably won’t happen as quickly as you think. I’ll leave with another post I wrote about that neuromyth.
High Proficiency in Weeks? It Can be Done, but not the Way you Think
Around mid-2014, while I was working at an accredited binational center in my city, I vividly remember watching a TEDx talk by a charismatic Irishman who called himself Benny, the Irish Polyglot. In fact, I was already familiar with Benny from my time at Cultura Inglesa, another excellent center I worked at. I had previously watched a video of him confidently and fluently speaking eight languages, including Portuguese. I loved sharing his videos with my adult students in an attempt to boost their motivation, and often, this strategy worked—at least for a while.
References
Abutalebi J (2008) Neural aspects of second language representation and language control. Acta Psychol 128: 466–478.
Andrews, E., Frigau, L., Voyvodic-Casabo, C., Voyvodic, J., & Wright, J. (2013). Multilingualism and fMRI: Longitudinal Study of Second Language Acquisition. Brain sciences, 3(2), 849-76. doi:10.3390/brainsci3020849
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax / Noam Chomsky. (Special technical report / MIT Research Laboratory of Electronics ; no.11). Cambridge, Mass.: M.I.T. Press.
Curtiss. (Perspectives in neurolinguistics and psycholinguistics). New York [etc.] ; London:Academic Press.
Ferman, S., Karni, A. (2010). No childhood advantage in the acquisition of skill in using an artificial language rule. PLoS ONE, 5(10), E13648.
Hartshorne, Tenenbaum, & Pinker. (2018). A critical period for second language acquisition:Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers. Cognition, 177, 263-277.
Hubel, D., & Wiesel, T. (1959). Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex. The Journal of Physiology, 148, 574-91.
Johnson, J.S., & Newport, E.L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning:
The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language.
Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60–99
Krashen, S. (1973). Lateralization, language learning, and the critical period. Language
Learning, 23, 63–74.
Kuhl PK, Ramírez RR, Bosseler A, Lin JF, Imada T. (2014). Infants' brain activity in response to speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Aug 2014, 111 (31) 11238-11245; DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1410963111
Lenneberg, Eric H. (1967). BIOLOGICAL FOUNDATIONS OF LANGUAGE. BIOLOGICAL FOUNDATIONS OF LANGUAGE., 1967.
Norrman, G., & Bylund, E. (2016). The irreversibility of sensitive period effects in language development: Evidence from second language acquisition in international adoptees. Developmental Science, 19(3), 513-520.
Penfield, W. (1959). The Interpretive Cortex. Science, 129(3365), 1719-1725.
Skinner, B. (1957). Verbal behavior / B.F. Skinner. (The Century psychology series). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.